Forum Lectures


July 9 6:00 p.m

Tracey Weldon

American Dialect Society Professor
University of South Carolina

That’s a Word! Middle Class African American English and the Significance of the Spoken Word
Despite decades of sociolinguistic research, most studies of African American English (AAE), have focused on working-class speech communities, largely at the exclusion of middle-class speakers. As a result, little consideration has been given to the linguistic dexterity of middle-class speakers and the extent to which they draw on the full range of the AAE continuum in the construction of their identities. In this talk, I explore some of the ways in which middle class speakers make use of vernacular structural features, as well as camouflaged features, and lexical and rhetorical expression, to strategically construct both race-based and class-based identities.


July 13th 6:00 p.m

Joan Bybee

Forum Lecturer
University of New Mexico

Usage-based Theory and Language Change
Conceiving of grammar as the cognitive representation of one’s experience with language means that common conceptions of how languages change need to be re-examined. Structuralist models require that change be an abrupt change in the grammar, which is often quite distanced from the details of language use. If cognitive representations contain details of usage, including phonetic detail as well as lexical and contextual information, all aspects of change can be gradual and can occur in language use. Moreover, if grammar is based on experience with language, then a separation of the individual from the community is not necessary, nor is it plausible to assume that speaker and listener play different roles in change. All language users have cognitive representations that reflect what they have heard and produced. Innovation itself is not an individual but a joint phenomenon since it arises from interaction among users who share common biases. These inherent biases make cross-generational directional change possible.


July 16th 6:00 p.m

Laura Michaelis

Fillmore Professor
University of Colorado, Boulder

Slouching toward Idiomaticity: Fillmore’s Rough Idea
In their 1988 magnum opus—known as the “let alone paper”—Fillmore and collaborators offer a pithy yet puzzling definition of idiom: “an idiomatic expression or construction is something a language user could fail to know while knowing everything else in the language” (p. 504). What, if any, is the difference between an idiomatic expression and a construction? The central insight of Let Alone is that language presents a continuum of idiomaticity, and that a construction grammar must capture expressions across this gradient—from frozen phrases to fully productive rules (Fillmore et al. 1988; Goldberg 1995, 2006; Wulff 2010; Culicover & Jackendoff 2005; Croft 2001; Hilpert 2014; Kay & Michaelis 2012). But is in the know—at one end—the same kind of grammatical object as the Head-Complement rule at the other?

Fillmore et al. (1988) list four conditions under which an expression can qualify as a construction: (1) it dependencies are not be limited to a mother and daughters, but span wider portions of the syntactic tree; (2) it specifies syntactic, lexical, semantic, and pragmatic information simultaneously; (3) it is a word or lexeme “mentionable in syntactic constructions”; and (4) it is idiomatic by virtue of conveying meaning “distinct from what might be calculated from the associated semantics of the set of smaller constructions that could be used to build the same morphosyntactic object” (p. 501). (Fillmore et al. 1988: 501). All such expressions constitute “conventional pairings between contexts and particular expressions by which conventionalized meanings get conveyed in those contexts” (Fillmore 1979: 72).

Let Alone and other works (e.g., Fillmore & Kay 1995, Kay & Fillmore 1999, Fillmore 1979, 1985, 2012, 2013) encouraged us to look at complex expressions through the lens of idiomaticity—to see words as constructions (both constellations of syntactic, phonological and pragmatic properties) and constructions as words (constructions license phrasal signs that mean what they mean in a word-like way). But these analogies obscured some fundamental differences: (a) many (if not most) idiomatic expressions are like sing a different tune, in having syntactically manipulable subparts (as in, e.g., Now a different tune is being sung), and are thus not realistically treated as ‘words with spaces’ (Kay et al. 2025); and (b) it strains credulity to assert that single signs (words and lexemes) are the same thing as combinations of signs (phrases). How can we strike a balance between ‘meaning by convention’ and


July 21st 7:00 p.m

Balthasar Bickel

Sapir Professor
University of Zurich

More information coming.


July 30th 6:00 p.m

Sonia Cristofaro

Collitz Professor
Sorbonne University

More information coming.


August 6th 6:00 p.m

Marianne Mithun

Hale Professor
UCSB

From Prosody toward Explanation?
There is now a rich and voluminous literature on relations between prosodic and syntactic structure, and interesting work continues to emerge. A basic observation is that speakers speak in spurts, or intonation units. Some models take syntactic structure as their point of departure and describe processes by which prosodic structure can be derived from it, considering how mismatches between syntactic constituents and prosodic units might be accounted for. Others begin with prosodic structure and consider how grammatical structure might emerge from it over time. With the second approach, it is generally agreed that syntactic and morphological structures tend to develop from the crystalization of frequently occurring sequences of adjacent elements, presumably within the same intonation unit. A closer look at languages which differ syntactically and morphologically from those on which much prosodic analysis has been based can help us to enrich our understanding of such processes. Prosody in Mohawk (Kanien’kéha’) reflects the organization of information at levels from discourse, through sentence-level syntax to fully crystalized morphology, with varying degrees of formal and functional categoriality. A close look at speaker choices in unscripted speech indicates that it is not simple adjacency within the intonation unit that shapes the gradual crystalization of structures, but rather the cognitive organization of ideas underlying the prosodic structure. The trajectory has not stopped with the development of morphology, however. The emergence of the rich morphology has in turn profoundly shaped modern syntactic structure.